Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 27 December 2016
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

The Contributions of Memory and Vocabulary to Non-Verbal Ability Scores in Adolescents with Intellectual Disability

\r\nChantanee MungkhetklangChantanee Mungkhetklang1Edith L. BavinEdith L. Bavin1Sheila G. Crewther*Sheila G. Crewther1*Nahal GoharpeyNahal Goharpey1Carl Parsons\r\nCarl Parsons2
  • 1School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
  • 2Port Phillip Specialist School, Port Melbourne, VIC, Australia

It is usually assumed that performance on non-verbal intelligence tests reflects visual cognitive processing and that aspects of working memory (WM) will be involved. However, the unique contribution of memory to non-verbal scores is not clear, nor is the unique contribution of vocabulary. Thus, we aimed to investigate these contributions. Non-verbal test scores for 17 individuals with intellectual disability (ID) and 39 children with typical development (TD) of similar mental age were compared to determine the unique contribution of visual and verbal short-term memory (STM) and WM and the additional variance contributed by vocabulary scores. No significant group differences were found in the non-verbal test scores or receptive vocabulary scores, but there was a significant difference in expressive vocabulary. Regression analyses indicate that for the TD group STM and WM (both visual and verbal) contributed similar variance to the non-verbal scores. For the ID group, visual STM and verbal WM contributed most of the variance to the non-verbal test scores. The addition of vocabulary scores to the model contributed greater variance for both groups. More unique variance was contributed by vocabulary than memory for the TD group, whereas for the ID group memory contributed more than vocabulary. Visual and auditory memory and vocabulary contributed significantly to solving visual non-verbal problems for both the TD group and the ID group. However, for each group, there were different weightings of these variables. Our findings indicate that for individuals with TD, vocabulary is the major factor in solving non-verbal problems, not memory, whereas for adolescents with ID, visual STM, and verbal WM are more influential than vocabulary, suggesting different pathways to achieve solutions to non-verbal problems.

Introduction

Cognitive ability in children is most often assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) (1). However, there are many situations where the language-based WISC could be argued to be less appropriate, e.g., when testing hearing and language impaired individuals and those with neurodevelopmental disorders, including intellectual disability (ID) (2) and/or autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (3), attention deficit/hyperactive disorder (4), or specific learning disorder (5). This has led to suggestions that non-verbal intelligence tests such as the Raven’s colored progressive matrices (RCPM) (6), the test of Non-verbal Intelligence-Fourth Edition (TONI-4) (7), the Comprehensive Test of Non-verbal Intelligence-Second Edition (8), the Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised (Leiter-R) (9), and the Wechsler Non-verbal Scale of Ability (WNV) (10) may be more appropriate. However, to date, there has been little research investigating the role verbal ability plays in visual problem solving tasks included in these tests or whether these tests adequately assess the cognitive abilities of individuals with ID, whose language is often impaired.

Intellectual disability is associated with delayed early development and severe problems with adaptive functioning (11), typically associated with limited cognitive abilities (1215). Currently, the most common diagnostic criterion for ID is an estimated intelligence Quotient (IQ) below 70 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) (11, 16, 17). Interestingly, despite the fact that the performance subtests from the WISC assume a degree of language for the child to understand and remember what the task requires and to respond appropriately (1, 18, 19), there has been comparatively little research investigating the impact of language and vocabulary on scores from IQ measures.

Wechsler reportedly viewed verbal and performance tests as equally valid measures of intelligence despite the fact that the Full Scale IQ score for the WISC is a combination of expressive vocabulary tasks and non-verbal problem solving (20). Non-verbal tests, used as alternative assessment tools of IQ, are assumed not to measure verbal skills, although the content and administration method of the non-verbal intelligence tests varies considerably (2125), and verbal instructions are often used. Some researchers have found that language influences the scores of non-verbal IQ tests (15, 26) and a significant association between performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and language measures (comprehension and naming) have been reported (27). In other research (28), both correct performance and error types on the RCPM were associated with receptive vocabulary scores from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT).

While there has been previous research with children with ID that has included receptive and expressive language measures [e.g., Ref. (15, 29, 30)], relatively little attention has been paid to directly exploring the unique contribution of vocabulary scores (expressive and receptive) to the non-verbal ability of individuals with ID.

Vocabulary development has been shown to be associated with verbal (phonological) short-term memory (STM) (3133) and, as argued by Gathercole and Alloway (34) verbal STM is particularly important in early vocabulary development, when there is little available support from existing lexical knowledge, which indicates the separability of vocabulary and STM (31). This view was supported in a study with children with down syndrome (35). However, while there has been research on STM in ID [e.g., Ref. (36, 37)], the unique variance contributed by expressive and receptive vocabulary to non-verbal IQ scores in addition to that contributed by STM (visual and spatial) and working memory (WM) has not been previously been examined. Identifying the unique contributions of memory and vocabulary to non-verbal assessment results will add to our understanding of the performance of individuals with ID on Non-verbal IQ tests. In previous research, IQ, STM, and WM have been shown to be associated (34, 3841), but they contribute differentially to cognitive abilities (4144), as illustrated by the reported correlation coefficients between WM and performance on tests of overall cognitive ability; they range from approximately 0.60 to 0.90.

There has been discussion in the literature of the phonological and visuospatial memory problems that individuals with ID seem to have with WM tasks (45), and deficits in both visual and verbal WM have been shown to increase with the severity of ID (46). Of interest is that visual facilitators (pictures and visual schematics) have been found to usefully assist development of more efficient memory protocols for individuals with ASD and ID (34), suggesting that visual processing ability compensates for deficits in verbal processing.

In the current study, we tested a group of adolescents with ID and a group of children with typical development (TD) to investigate the extent to which expressive and receptive vocabulary scores add to the variance contributed by STM and WM to their non-verbal IQ scores. Three tests, the RCPM, the TONI-4, and the WNV, were chosen on the basis of the visual problem solving content and limited requirements for verbal expression (either in administration and/or responses). All three tests were designed to have a low degree of cultural loading and linguistic demands (22), and scores on these three tests have been found to provide a similar mental age as the WISC-IV for 5–7-year-old children with TD and adolescents with ID aged 11–16 years (47).

The specific aims of the study were to enhance understanding of the non-verbal ability of adolescents with ID. We compared a group of individuals with ID and a group of children with TD of comparable mental age in order to determine the following:

 i. The amount of variance contributed to non-verbal test scores by auditory and visual STM and WM.

ii. The amount of additional variance contributed to non-verbal scores by expressive and receptive vocabulary scores.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The 56 participants were 17 adolescents with ID ranging in age from 11 years 4 months to 16 years 7 months (5 females, 12 males, M = 13.57, SD = 1.28) and 39 children with TD ranging in age from 5 years 4 months to 7 years 11 months (16 females, 23 males, M = 6.35, SD = 0.76). Of the ID participants, eight had a diagnosis of ASD, three had attention deficit and hyperactive disorder, two had idiopathic ID, and one had Williams syndrome; and three had genetic disorder diagnoses, i.e., congenital occipital encephalocele, galactosemia (thalamic stroke), and fetal anticonvulsant syndrome. None of the children with TD had a diagnosis of any neurological or cognitive disorder. We compared the ID group with a group of school children with TD aged 5–7 years, an age expected to be equivalent to the mean mental age of the ID group.

Parents/guardians provided informed consent prior to their child’s participation. All individuals were screened for normal hearing and vision. Ethics approval was obtained from the La Trobe University Human Ethics Committee (HEC approval number: HEC06-036 and FHEC13/R22) and the State Department of Education (Approval number: 2012-001425). Permission to conduct testing in the schools was obtained from the school principals. Parents/guardians provided informed consent prior to their child’s participation.

Materials

Non-Verbal Tests

Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices

The RCPM is a standardized test of non-verbal intelligence. The RCPM measures fluid ability, that is, the ability to form comparisons, to reason by analogy, and to organize spatial perceptions into a systematically related whole. It was developed for use with individuals with TD aged 5 years 11 months to the elderly, as well as the mentally and physically impaired. Each item consists of an array of colored patterns with a missing portion, and the examinee is required to choose the missing elements from a group of six possible options. Split-half reliabilities range from 0.65 to 0.94. Concurrent validity coefficients between Raven’s progressive matrices and other intelligence tests are in the 0.50 to 0.80 s (6).

Test of Non-Verbal Intelligence-Fourth Edition

The TONI-4 is a standardized test of non-verbal intelligence. Non-verbal items of the standard figures matrices type were used to develop the TONI as an assessment of the cognitive ability of children and adults. The assessment is designed for use with individuals aged 6 years 0 months to 89 years 11 months. Each item is a black and white pattern with a missing portion. Items are in a multiple choice format, with either five or six response alternatives. Instructions are given via pantomime; individuals respond by pointing, nodding, or blinking. Internal consistency reliability ranges from 0.94 to 0.97. Correlation coefficients with other non-verbal intelligence tests range from 0.73 to 0.79 (48).

Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability

The WNV is a standardized test of non-verbal intelligence. It is designed for use with TD individuals aged 4 years 0 months to 21 years 11 months. The WNV comprises seven subtests, the particular ones administered depending on the age of the examinee: 4 years 0 month to 7 years 11 months or 8 years 0 months to 21 years 11 months. The WNV subtests for the younger age group were used in this study, that is, Matrices, Coding A, Object Assembly, and Recognition. The Matrices subtest requires the examinee to identify how different geometric shapes are spatially or logically interrelated and to complete the relationship among the parts. The test items are varied (e.g., geometric pattern, reasoning by analogy, and spatial visualization). The displays use basic geometric figures, such as squares, circles, and triangles, and use some combination of colors: black, white, yellow, blue, and green. The Coding subtest requires the examinee to co-operate with verbal instructions to copy symbols (e.g., dash, two vertical lines, and an open parenthesis) that are paired with simple geometric shapes according to a key provided at the top of the page. The Object assembly subtest consists of items that require the examinee to complete pieces of a puzzle to form a recognizable object such as a ball or a car. The Recognition subtest requires the examinee to examine a stimulus (e.g., a square with a small circle in the center) for 3 s and then choose which of four or five options is identical to the stimulus that was just seen. Average internal consistency reliability for the Full Scale IQ on four subtests versions are 0.91. Correlations between the four-subtest version Full Scale IQ and other test of intelligence range between 0.71 and 0.82 (10).

Vocabulary Tests

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth Edition (PPVT-4)

We used the PPVT-4 to measure receptive vocabulary. It is used for individuals aged 2 years 6 months to 90+ years. The examinee is required to choose from a set of four pictures, the one that best relates to the word said aloud by the examiner. Correlation coefficients with the Expressive Vocabulary Test-Second Edition (EVT-2) average 0.82 and with other tests range from 0.41 to 0.79 (49).

Expressive Vocabulary Test-Second Edition

The EVT-2 was used as the assessment of expressive vocabulary. It is designed for individuals aged 2 years 6 months to 90+ years old. The examinee labels the pictures aloud, e.g., when shown a picture of a ball, the expected answer is “ball.” Correlation coefficients with the PPVT-4 scale average 0.82 and with other tests range from 0.50 to 0.81 (50). The PPVT and EVT-2 are co-normed, allowing direct comparisons between their respective standard scores.

Memory Tasks

Digit Span Visual and Verbal Tasks

Our measure of STM was forward digit span. Backward digit span, which requires manipulation of the information presented, was the measure of WM. In order to establish consistent presentation of the stimuli, a VPixx computer program (51) was developed to present the Digit Span tasks—with both visual and verbal presentations of numbers. Participants were required to complete both visual and verbal version of the tasks. The order in which the tasks were given was counterbalanced. For the verbal tasks, participants listened to the numbers presented through the computer sound system at a rate of 1/s. Two trials per span length (i.e., the number of digits in the given sequence) were provided; when two trials of the same span length were answered incorrectly testing was discontinued. For the visual task, a list of digits (1–9) was presented on a computer screen at a rate of 1/s.

Procedure

The participants were tested individually at school. All tests were conducts using standardized instructions, scoring, and interpretation. The results for each participant were considered a valid assessment, not impeded by behavioral or emotional factors. Test sessions lasted 15–45 min, depending on each participant’s concentration span.

Analysis

To compare groups on their scores for each of the non-verbal assessments, the two vocabulary and four memory measures, one-way ANOVAs between groups were used. A regression analysis for each non-verbal test identified the amount of variance contributed by each of the memory measures. In a second series of regression analyses, the vocabulary scores were added (together with the memory scores) to identify the additional variance contributed by expressive and receptive vocabulary to each of the three non-verbal IQ test scores.

Results

Scores from each non-verbal test and the PPVT and EVT were first checked by group for normality; the results met all expected criteria. Descriptive statistics (the mean and SD of the raw scores for each test) for the three non-verbal tests for each group are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and raw scores for the TD and ID groups.

One-way ANOVAs between groups were conducted on scores from each of the three non-verbal tests. There were no significant group differences on scores for the RCPM, F (1, 54) = 0.498, p = 0.483, Cohen’s d = 0.19, nor for the TONI-4, F (1, 54) = 1.564, p = 0.216, Cohen’s d = 0.35, nor for the WNV, F (1, 54) = 0.029, p = 0.866, Cohen’s d = 0.04. The ID and TD groups performed similarly on each of the three tests, indicating that the non-verbal mental age of each group was similar, as expected. However, there was a large effect size for the TONI-4.

One-way ANOVAs were also conducted on the expressive and receptive vocabulary test scores to compare performance of the ID and TD groups. Although there were no significant group differences on the PPVT-IV raw scores, F (1, 54) = 0.451, p = 0.505, Cohen’s d = 0.17, there was a significant group difference on the EVT-2 raw scores, F (1, 54) = 11.48, p = 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.91. The EVT-2 scores were lower for the ID group than the TD group.

For the visual and verbal digit span forward and backward tasks, the TD group mean scores were higher than for the ID group. One-way ANOVAs between groups were conducted on each task. There was a significant group difference on the digit span backward visual task, F (1, 54) = 4.354, p = 0.042, Cohen’s d = 0.59, with a large effect size. However, there were no significant group differences for digit span forward visual, F (1, 54) = 1.056, p = 0.309, Cohen’s d = 0.27, digit span forward verbal, F (1, 54) = 0.161, p = 0.690, Cohen’s d = 0.10, digit span backward verbal, F (1, 54) = 2.160, p = 0.147, Cohen’s d = 0.40, although the effect size for the latter was large.

Regression analyses on the scores from the three non-verbal tests were first conducted with the two STM scores as predictor variables, that is, visual STM and verbal STM. In a second regression analysis, the two WM tasks were included as predictor variables. Table 2 shows the results. For the TD group, visual and verbal STM did not contribute significant variance to any of the three non-verbal test scores. Visual STM contributed only ~7% to the RCPM and the WNV and ~3% to the TONI-4; verbal STM contributed ~3% to the RCPM, ~7% to the TONI-4, and ~9% to the WNV. In contrast, visual WM contributed significant variance to two of the non-verbal tests: 10.5 and 12.5%, respectively, to the TONI-4 and the WNV, but no significant variance to the RCPM (~5%). By comparison, verbal WM contributed significant variance to the RCPM (10.5%), but not to the TONI-4 (~3%) and WNV (~7%).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results from the multiple regression analysis using memory scores for TD group and ID group.

For the ID group, visual STM contributed significant variance to all three non-verbal test scores: 46.2% to the RCPM, 29.3% to the TONI-4, and 32.6% to the WNV, but as for the TD group, verbal STM did not contribute significant variance to any of the non-verbal test scores. In contrast to the TD results, visual WM contributed significant variance (23.6%) to the RCPM, but not to the TONI-4 (21.3%) or the WNV (21.7%). In addition, verbal WM contributed significant variance to all three non-verbal test scores: 34.4% to the RCPM, 26.9% to the TONI-4, and 25% to the WNV.

In order to determine the amount of variance contributed by Vocabulary over and above that contributed by Memory, the Vocabulary scores were added as predictor variables in a second set of regression analyses. As shown in Table 3, for the TD group with EVT-2 scores added, for all four memory measures there was a significant increase in the variance. Approximately two to three times more variance was found than previously to the RCPM and the TONI-4, and even more to the WNV. The unique variance for expressive vocabulary was greater than for memory for all three non-verbal tests. By comparison, for the ID group while the EVT-2 scores increased the variance for all three non-verbal test scores, with visual STM, vocabulary contributed significant unique variance for the RCPM and WN. In contrast to the TD group, when EVT-2 scores were included with verbal STM, no significant unique variance was contributed by vocabulary. With visual WM and EVT-2 scores, expressive vocabulary contributed significant unique variance only to the RCPM; a similar result was found when EVT-2 scores and verbal WM were included in the regression. The unique variance for memory for the ID group was larger than for vocabulary in all four analyses.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Variance contributed to non-verbal test scores with vocabulary in addition to memory scores.

The addition of PPVT scores rather than EVT-2 scores for the TD group showed a similar pattern overall as for the EVT-2. The unique variance contributed by the PPVT was greater than for Memory. With visual STM significant unique variance from receptive vocabulary was found for the Toni and WVN but not for the RCPM. With verbal STM and also with visual WM, similar results were found. However, for verbal WM, receptive vocabulary contributed significant unique variance to all three non-verbal scores. For the ID group, with visual STM, significant unique variance was contributed by receptive vocabulary to all three non-verbal tests but more unique variance was contributed by Memory than Vocabulary. With verbal STM, a significant contribution from receptive vocabulary was found only for the RCPM. With visual WM, no significant contribution from receptive vocabulary was found for any of the three non-verbal tests, but with verbal WM there was a significant contribution from receptive vocabulary for the RCPM. That is, vocabulary scores added to the variance when they were included with the memory scores, but the variance contributed differed for each of the three non-verbal tests.

Discussion

The results overall provide evidence that memory (both STM and WM) and vocabulary (both expressive and receptive vocabulary) are associated with performance on non-verbal tests of IQ, both for children with TD and adolescents with ID of the same non-verbal mental age.

As expected there were no significant group differences on the three non-verbal test scores, although the mean scores of the ID group were slightly higher, and there was greater variability on all three non-verbal tests for the ID group than for the TD group and with a large effect size on the TONI. Nor was there a significant group difference for receptive vocabulary, but the expressive vocabulary scores differed with the younger, TD group achieving significantly higher scores than the adolescent ID group. For the memory tasks, there was a group difference on the visual WM task; given the large effect sizes on both WM tasks it is likely that with a larger sample of individuals with ID, significant group differences would have been found for both the verbal and visual WM tasks.

No evidence for a significant relationship between STM and non-verbal problem solving was found for the TD group, whereas visual STM contributed significantly to all three non-verbal test scores for the ID group. This indicates that the participants with ID were more likely to be successful in problem solving on the non-verbal tasks if they had higher visual STM scores and that visual STM played a major role in their problem solving strategy.

In contrast, both visual WM and verbal WM contributed variance to the three non-verbal tests for the TD group but differently depending on the test involved. For solving the RCPM tasks, verbal WM was important. This indicates that the young children were rehearsing as they identified features of the patterns they needed to complete and held these in memory as they compared the alternatives in order to reach a decision on how best to complete the matrices. For the other two non-verbal tests, visual WM was more important for solving the problems. However, for older children than in our study (i.e., over 7 years), verbal WM is likely to be more involved since memory develops over the primary school years (52, 53).

Both visual WM and verbal WM played a role in solving the RCPM tasks for the ID group, but for solving tasks on the other two non-verbal tests verbal WM but not visual WM contributed significantly. Based on these results, non-verbal assessments cannot be viewed as “language free.” Success on solving the problems for individuals with ID depends on using their verbal WM. Of interest is that for both groups receptive and expressive vocabulary scores added significant variance to that contributed by memory to the non-verbal test scores for both groups but less so for the ID group.

Our findings clearly show that vocabulary (expressive and receptive) will impact on how well a child with TD in the early school years performs on non-verbal ability tests. The results show a greater contribution from vocabulary than memory when they are solving non-verbal problems. The WNV, in particular, showed a greater influence of vocabulary for the TD group. Four tasks of a different nature, drawing on different cognitive skills, make up the assessment, and we assume this had some influence on the result. In contrast, the results for the ID group support the view that visual STM as well as visual and verbal WM play a more important role than vocabulary in completing non-verbal tasks for adolescents with ID. Visual STM had a greater influence than verbal STM or WM (visual or verbal) on their non-verbal scores.

The study contributes to the literature by comparing performance on three non-verbal tests and considering the unique contributions of memory and vocabulary to the assessment results. However, the study is not without limitations. The ID participants were all recruited from one special school, and there were multiple diagnoses. The sample size was not large and could have been increased by extending recruitment to other schools. Even so, significant results were found. However, the results need to be replicated with larger samples from different educational environments. In past research testing intelligence, memory and language, principal components analysis identified that more variance loaded onto a fluid reasoning factor (i.e., higher order thinking skills) for younger children with TD than older children (31). Thus, while we were interested in comparing groups of similar mental age, inclusion of a TD group of comparable chronological age as the ID group could have provided valuable information about the roles of memory and vocabulary in problem solving for tasks included in non-verbal assessments of IQ.

Conclusion

Our study has demonstrated that both TD and ID individuals draw on different abilities to achieve the same result when solving non-verbal problems. The TD group drew more on their vocabulary knowledge, while the ID apparently relied more on visual memory. Even though the ID were chronologically much older and likely to have had greater experience of language, when compared with the TD group their expressive vocabulary was significantly lower. Thus, adolescents with ID are likely to achieve low scores on assessments (including non-verbal assessments) that may or may not use verbal instructions or require verbal responses. Our current findings indicate that more research on the interface between memory and language skills in relation to cognitive ability will be beneficial in helping develop appropriate ways of assessing the cognitive abilities and development of individuals with ID. In additions, a focus on training visual STM for young people with ID is recommended.

Author Contributions

CM collected the data, conducted the analyses and interpretation of the data, and prepared early drafts of the paper. SC and EB conceptualized and supervised development of the research, assisted with analysis and interpretation of the data, and contributed to writing the manuscript. NG participated in the collection of the data and helped manuscript preparation. CP helped in recruitment and provide testing facilities. All the authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding

The first author was supported by a Royal Thai Government PhD Scholarship, and additional funds were provided by the School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University. ARC DP0985837 Crewther SG, Crewther DP – The advantage of being magnocellular: the role of the dorsal visual stream in object identification.

References

1. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (2003).

Google Scholar

2. Orsini A, Pezzuti L, Hulbert S. Beyond the floor effect on the Wechsler intelligence scale for children – 4th Ed. (WISC-IV): calculating IQ and indexes of subjects presenting a floored pattern of results. J Intellect Disabil Res (2015) 59(5):468–73. doi:10.1111/jir.12150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nader A-M, Courchesne V, Dawson M, Soulières I. Does WISC-IV underestimate the intelligence of autistic children? J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46(5):1582–9. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2270-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. WISC-IV and WISC-III profiles in children with ADHD. J Atten Disord (2006) 9(3):486–93. doi:10.1177/1087054705283616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Styck KM, Watkins MW. Structural validity of the WISC-IV for students with learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil (2016) 49(2):216–24. doi:10.1177/0022219414539565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Raven JC. Colored Progressive Matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press (1995).

Google Scholar

7. Fopiano J. Test of nonverbal intelligence (TONI-4). In: Volkmar F, editor. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York: Springer (2013). p. 3089–92.

Google Scholar

8. Hammill DD, Pearson NA, Weiderholt JL. Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence. 2nd ed. Austin: PRO-ED (2009).

Google Scholar

9. Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter International Performance Scale-Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting (1997).

Google Scholar

10. Wechsler D, Naglieri JA. Wechsler Nonverbal Scale of Ability. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment (2006).

Google Scholar

11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

12. Bonifacci P, Snowling MJ. Speed of processing and reading disability: a cross-linguistic investigation of dyslexia and borderline intellectual functioning. Cognition (2008) 107(3):999–1017. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Carulla LS, Reed GM, Vaez-Azizi LM, Cooper S-A, Leal RM, Bertelli M, et al. Intellectual developmental disorders: towards a new name, definition and framework for “mental retardation/intellectual disability” in ICD-11. World Psychiatry (2011) 10(3):175–80. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00045.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cornoldi C, Giofrè D, Orsini A, Pezzuti L. Differences in the intellectual profile of children with intellectual vs. learning disability. Res Dev Disabil (2014) 35(9):2224–30. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. van der Schuit M, Segers E, van Balkom H, Verhoeven L. How cognitive factors affect language development in children with intellectual disabilities. Res Dev Disabil (2011) 32(5):1884–94. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

17. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000).

Google Scholar

18. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. New York: The Psychological Corporation (1949).

Google Scholar

19. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation (1974).

Google Scholar

20. Boake C. From the Binet–Simon to the Wechsler–Bellevue: tracing the history of intelligence testing. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2002) 24(3):383–405. doi:10.1076/jcen.24.3.383.981

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. DeThorne LS, Schaefer BA. A guide to child nonverbal IQ measures. Am J Speech Lang Pathol (2004) 13(4):275–90. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2004/029)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. McCallum RS. Assessing intelligence nonverbally. In: Bracken BA, Geisinger KF, editors. APA Handbook of Testing and Assessment in Psychology, Vol 3: Testing and Assessment in School Psychology and Education. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2013). p. 71–99.

Google Scholar

23. Naglieri JA, Otero TM. Assessing diverse populations with nonverbal measures of ability in a neuropsychological context. In: Davis A, editor. Handbook of Pediatric Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Co (2011). p. 227–34.

Google Scholar

24. Russell R, Stokes J, Jones M, Czogalik D, Rohleder L. The role of nonverbal sensitivity in childhood psychopathology. J Nonverbal Behav (1993) 17(1):69–83. doi:10.1007/bf00987009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Verhaegh J, Fontijn WFJ, Resing WCM. On the correlation between children’s performances on electronic board tasks and nonverbal intelligence test measures. Comput Educ (2013) 69:419–30. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2013.07.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Christy EM, Friedman RB. Using non-verbal tests to measure cognitive ability in patients with aphasia: a comparison of the RCPM and the TONI. Brain Lang (2005) 95(1):195–6. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2005.07.104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Baldo JV, Dronkers NF, Wilkins D, Ludy C, Raskin P, Kim J. Is problem solving dependent on language? Brain Lang (2005) 92(3):240–50. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Goharpey N, Crewther DP, Crewther SG. Problem solving ability in children with intellectual disability as measured by the Raven’s colored progressive matrices. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(12):4366–74. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Cascella PW. Standardised speech-language tests and students with intellectual disability: a review of normative data. J Intellect Dev Disabil (2006) 31(2):120–4. doi:10.1080/13668250600681503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Garrity LI, Donoghue JT. Preschool children’s performance on the Raven’s coloured progressive matrices and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Educ Psychol Meas (1976) 36(4):1043–7. doi:10.1177/001316447603600433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Archibald LMD. The language, working memory, and other cognitive demands of verbal tasks. Top Lang Disord (2013) 33(3):190–207. doi:10.1097/TLD.0b013e31829dd8af

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Baddeley A. Working memory and language: an overview. J Commun Disord (2003) 36(3):189–208. doi:10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hick RF, Botting N, Conti-Ramsden G. Short-term memory and vocabulary development in children with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol (2005) 47(8):532–8. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01187.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gathercole SE, Alloway TP. Practitioner review: short-term and working memory impairments in neurodevelopmental disorders: diagnosis and remedial support. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2006) 47(1):4–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01446.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Brock J, Jarrold C. Language influences on verbal short-term memory performance in down syndrome: item and order recognition. J Speech Lang Hear Res (2004) 47(6):1334–46. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2004/100)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Hewes AK. Genetically dissociated components of working memory: evidence from downs and williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia (1999) 37(6):637–51. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00128-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Hewes AK. Verbal Short-term Memory Deficits in Down Syndrome: A Consequence of Problems in Rehearsal? J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2000) 41(2):233–44. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Dang C-P, Braeken J, Colom R, Ferrer E, Liu C. Why is working memory related to intelligence? Different contributions from storage and processing. Memory (2014) 22(4):426–41. doi:10.1080/09658211.2013.797471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway ARA. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. J Exp Psychol Gen (1999) 128(3):309–31. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Giofrè D, Mammarella IC. The relationship between working memory and intelligence in children: is the scoring procedure important? Intelligence (2014) 46:300–10. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Giofrè D, Mammarella IC, Cornoldi C. The structure of working memory and how it relates to intelligence in children. Intelligence (2013) 41(5):396–406. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Henry L, Winfield J. Working memory and educational achievement in children with intellectual disabilities. J Intellectu Disabil Res (2010) 54(4):354–65. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01264.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lervåg A, Bråten I, Hulme C. The cognitive and linguistic foundations of early reading development: a Norwegian latent variable longitudinal study. Dev Psychol (2009) 45(3):764–81. doi:10.1037/a0014132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Montgomery J, Magimairaj B, O’Malley M. Role of working memory in typically developing children’s complex sentence comprehension. J Psycholinguist Res (2008) 37(5):331–54. doi:10.1007/s10936-008-9077-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Rosenquist C, Conners FA, Roskos-Ewoldsen B. Phonological and visuo-spatial working memory in individuals with intellectual disability. Am J Ment Retard (2003) 108(6):403–13. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<403:PAVWMI>2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Schuchardt K, Gebhardt M, Mäehler C. Working memory functions in children with different degrees of intellectual disability. J Intellect Disabil Res (2010) 54(4):346–53. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01265.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Mungkhetklang C, Crewther SG, Bavin EL, Goharpey N, Parsons C. Comparison of measures of ability in adolescents with intellectual disability. Front Psychol (2016) 7:683. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Brown L, Sherbenou RJ, Johnsen SK. Test of Nonverbal Intelligence. 4th ed. Austin, TX: PRO-ED (2010).

Google Scholar

49. Dunn LM, Dunn DM, American Guidance S. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth Edition (PPVT-4). Bloomington, MN: NCS: Pearson Assessments (2007).

Google Scholar

50. Williams KT, Pearson Education I. Expressive Vocabulary Test-Second Edition (EVT-2). USA: Pearson Assessments (2007).

Google Scholar

51. VPixx Program (Version 2.87) [Computer Program]. Saint-Bruno, QC: Vpixx Technologies, Inc. (2006).

Google Scholar

52. Cowan N, Alloway TP. The development of working memory in childhood. In: Cowan E, editor. Development of Memory in Childhood. Hove, England: Psychology Press (2009). p. 303–42.

Google Scholar

53. Hornung C, Brunner M, Reuter RAP, Martin R. Children’s working memory: its structure and relationship to fluid intelligence. Intelligence (2011) 39(4):210–21. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: intellectual disability, non-verbal IQ tests, short-term memory, working memory, vocabulary

Citation: Mungkhetklang C, Bavin EL, Crewther SG, Goharpey N and Parsons C (2016) The Contributions of Memory and Vocabulary to Non-Verbal Ability Scores in Adolescents with Intellectual Disability. Front. Psychiatry 7:204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00204

Received: 24 August 2016; Accepted: 13 December 2016;
Published: 27 December 2016

Edited by:

Nora S. Vyas, Kingston University, UK

Reviewed by:

Munis Dundar, Erciyes University, Turkey
Om Sai Ramesh Vallamkonda, Lady Hardinge Medical College, India
Yasuo Terao, University of Tokyo, Japan

Copyright: © 2016 Mungkhetklang, Bavin, Crewther, Goharpey and Parsons. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sheila G. Crewther, s.crewther@latrobe.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.